Hacker News new | past | comments | ask | show | jobs | submit login
The Brilliance of a Stradivari Violin Might Rest Within Its Wood (nytimes.com)
62 points by leephillips on Dec 21, 2016 | hide | past | favorite | 97 comments



Does anyone know if the researchers have made an effort to quantify the acoustics they are trying to recreate or explain?

The NYTimes article summarizes it this way: "For hundreds of years, the best violin players have almost unanimously said they prefer a Stradivari or a Guarneri instrument... Why nobody has been able to replicate that sound remains one of the most enduring mysteries of instrument building.

Here's the thing, is there actually a notably different sound? I have read the study that shows professional musicians don't necessarily prefer the sound of a stradivarius over a high quality new instrument, that doesn't necessarily mean they don't sound different. How good are they at telling the difference between these instruments, is it something a non-musician could notice or be trained to notice relatively easily.

Ideally, a researcher would be able to quantify the tone that is elusive. Alternatively, a team of listeners who can clearly distinguish between the two, reliable, would be useful. Because it sounds like the goal here isn't so much to get a really nice sound, it's to figure out why these instruments sound different (if indeed they do!)

I read the abstract, and it sounds like they've analyzed the composition of the violins and have found some notable chemical differences in the wood. It's not an uninteresting result, but I still have to wonder if we're a few steps ahead of ourselves here. First things first, do Stradivarius instruments, as a group, have an identifiably different tone or acoustic quality that makes it possible, either through acoustic measurements or trained human evaluation, to reliably classify them apart from other instruments.


I linked to this study in another thread here, but they tested whether professional violinists could tell whether a violin was old or new. The conclusion for this test is: no, they can't reliably hear the difference between Strads and modern instruments.

http://www.thestrad.com/blind-tested-soloists-unable-to-tell...

"Soloists readily distinguished instruments they liked from those they did not but were unable to tell old from new at better than chance levels,’ the report concluded. ‘Given the stature and experience of our soloists, continuing claims for the existence of playing qualities unique to old Italian violins are strongly in need of empirical support."


My wife is a professional violinist. (In Australia at least) if you've watched the news or a recent Australia movie, you would have heard her play.

She says you can ALWAYS tell the difference. But she also says, "better" depends on the music. If you are playing a song on X-Factor or Australian Idol or even Christmas carols, then you'll never notice. If you are playing a solo for a classical piece (wedding or other ceremony), then it certainly sounds "different".

Better is debatable, but different, yes


Eh, I'm going to question her ability to identify the difference between a Strad and non-Strad based on this double blind study[1]. There's a lot of fetishism in the community on the Strad, probably to the joy of Christie's auctioneers worldwide. "The old growth trees in the region can't be sourced anymore". "no, it's the varnish. Your resonance is affected by both the chemical composition of the mix as well as the way it aged with the wood".

I'd imagine if 5 of the best sawyers with 5 of the best luthiers and sound engineers in the world all sat down together with the goal of replicating the Strad's sound with 100% accuracy I'm confident the 'unique' sound of the Strad could be replicated. It might take a lot of time and maybe a few hundred k in equipment (a dozen condenser mics placed strategically at a variety of places within the room and near the instrument itself, some vibration analysis equipment on the equipment, etc), but eventually the sawyer will choose the right wood, the engineer would perform acoustic analysis and tell the luthier "ok, that 43 micron chisel shave you just took just did __, 22 more and we match the F# perfectly on the G string".

I think it's very similar to the whole audiophile thing, where at least some of it is placebo. Those $30 monster cables aren't transmitting with any more signal fidelity than your $5 ones (assuming equal gauge copper, proper ohmic termination, blah blah), and I can sit down and prove it to anyone with $400 worth of spec-ans from the 1980s.

Now the "different" vs "better" -- I love my record player but when audiophiles say 'it sounds better' than the raw mix-down digital masters, in terms of the amount of audible audio content objectively this isn't the case. (Edit: See [4]) It sounds different from what you're used to. You like the 'warmth' that a belt drive record through a 1950s speaker produces. You like the hisses and crackles and pops. It's an emotional connection you have. When I was playing guitar with my crappy band, I'd run my digital content through a Tascam quarter inch tape deck just to get that hiss in the tracks before we sent it down for mastering (we didn't have those fancy plug-ins to do it for us then).

The same thing happened with film.[2] TV is generally shot at 30fps, film at 24fps[3]. When the FPS rate increased with the advent of more modern technology and higher sampling rates, people would claim that cinema didn't look 'cinematic'. The motion blur of the analog film experience we grew up with (well, those of us over 25) is something we mentally associated with the experience of going to the cinema. You saw a bunch of people comment on this with the Hobbit being distributed and shown at theatres at 48 FPS. "It just doesn't look right".

--

[1]http://www.thestrad.com/blind-tested-soloists-unable-to-tell...

[2] I'm sorry I don't have a source on this study, but the phenomenon was pretty widespread. Cinema isn't something I've delved too far into but the cinematographer forums have thousands (literally) of threads on FPS discussion.

[3] That's just historically how things broke down -- 24 was a convenient number to match up with actual seconds due to it's easy factorability, so you could get half a second at 12 frames, a quarter at 6, etc. This made it easy back in the day when post-processing involved actually involving cutting nitrate-based film and sound syncing to the video involved physically matching the snap of the clapper on your audio track to the visual of the actual frame of when the hinge closed.

RE: 30 fps, I'm not sure, but I'd guess it had to do with the fact that we're at 60 hertz in the US so you could interlace half a frame a cycle (ie. trigger on the peak of every AC cycle to update the set of horizontal lines modulo two, so the electron gun presumably only had to hit half the lines within that time constraint) Other parts of the world operate at 50hz, which is why I'd imagine you have the PAL standard and 25 fps in those locales. (Pure speculation though, someone who's knows for sure, jump in.)

[4] Operating under the assumption that that they were pressed from the same master source.. I'm limited only to the records I bought which were from bands with too small of a budget to afford two separate masters. (I.e., That mid-90s emo 200 run EP by Knapsack didn't have two masters, hell on tour they were lucky to have a floor to crash on.) Still, poster beneath me makes a very valid point that should be taken into consideration (Though I'm sure you're already aware of whether or not the band had a separate master DAT/master plate made if you like the band enough to care about the nuanced differences between two masters.)


>I love my record player but when audiophiles say 'it sounds better' than the raw mix-down digital masters, in terms of the amount of audible audio content objectively this isn't the case. It sounds different from what you're used to.

There is a case to be made that CERTAIN albums will produce an objectively better sound in several respects when compared to the CD/digital version of the master. This is an effect of the "Loudness Wars" that began in the late 80's and still continues, albeit at a lower intensity, today.

The analog nature of the vinyl format places a hard limit on how "hot" one could make the sound by tweaking/compressing the dynamic range. Eventually the mastering would exceed the physical ability of the needle to track the groove which is obviously untenable for everyone involved. CD's on the other hand, can push the range as far as acoustically possible and people who want their new rock album to really ROCK are going to be more impressed by a master that somehow sounds louder than any other album at a given volume setting. Thus, the Loudness Wars began and have progressed to the point where the dynamic range has been pushed into the realm of guaranteed acoustic clipping because it still sounds louder and the loss of fidelity won't matter when the song is blasted from an iPhone speaker.

Obviously, this is an effect limited to specific albums. Even if you buy one a pressing of one of these albums, you aren't guaranteed better sound as the vinyl boom has led to some really half-ass remasters that can be as half-assed as passing the borked digital master through conversion software to ensure it works without making any further effort.

Aside from this one outlier scenario, a song/album/whatever mastered attentively to the CD format will always deliver objectively better sound quality.

Source: http://www.soundmattersblog.com/vinyl-vs-cd-in-the-loudness-...


Spot on. Mastering for a wider dynamic range on vinyl makes sense. Not only because of the inherent physical limitations, but it's also reasonable to expect the listener has a decent sound system to go with the record player. It's also common for level to vary between records. Wider grooves allow for more loudness, so it's a tradeoff versus running time and playback speed.

Fortunately, the shift from CDs to digital as primary target has brought an end to the Loudness War. The various services have standards for perceived loudness, so there's no incentive for the mastering engineer to squeeze a few extra decibels out of the mix - past a certain point you're accomplishing nothing but killing your headroom margin.


I'd imagine if 5 of the best sawyers with 5 of the best luthiers and sound engineers in the world all sat down together with the goal of replicating the Strad's sound with 100% accuracy I'm confident the 'unique' sound of the Strad could be replicated. It'd might take a lot of time and maybe a few hundred k in equipment (a dozen condensor mics placed strategically at a variety of places within the room and near the instrument itself, some vibration analysis equipment on the equipment, etc), but eventually the sawyer will choose the right wood, the engineer would identify "ok, that 43 micron chisel shave you just took just did __, 22 more and we match the F# perfectly on the G string".

Indeed, the behavior of violins has been the subject of intensive study for decades. Every new technique for measuring sound or vibration is applied to violins. There's an article every few years about some new secret discovered in the great fiddles. Good acoustic measurement gear is now more sensitive than human hearing. It was only a matter of time before somebody cracked the code.

An amusing rumor is that the tone of a violin changes over time, due to age and playing, and that the Strads are in decline.


To be fair, a violin's tone does change over time especially with playing. Take the cylinders in your car. Every time that piston moves up and down, it wears away a little bit at the cylinder rings and a little bit on the cylinder walls. Particulate masses may collect in the bore. If you're running diesel, there are micro-cavitations literally eroding away the inner-bore of that cylinder. Air filters might get dirty, fuel lines clogged, manifolds warped, hydraulic pressure in your break calipers will vary. All of these ultimately effecting why your car handles differently than when it came out of the factory.

The same thing is true for the fingerboard, the bridge, the strings and other components you interface with (or are interfaced with from things with which you interface) on your violin. As the oils on your finger on the strings/fingerboard gradually acts as basically a mini-abrasive tool, the fingerboard itself will change shape (you can see this easily in the wear patterns). The pressure variance from the string's depression, the shifts in temperature/humidity in the ambient environment (which will stress the wood in all sorts of ways, both in an elastic (temporary) as well as a plastic (permanent) manner), all of these micro-variables add up over time, just like with your car.


Another effect is that our hands and ears change over time. I'm not sure I'd be capable of conducting an objective before-after test of my instrument over the seven years that I've owned it. My technique has changed, I've changed the strings, my bow is due to be re-haired, etc. And my ears are seven years older.


The study in [1] has a sample size of 10 in Paris, in one setting. I'd hardly call that conclusive or ruling out all the possible confounding variables.


Also known as "I am convinced this false thing is true, and will selectively demand higher levels of rigor from anything opposing my view than I ever have from anything supporting my view".

I've read quite a bit about the blind violin studies, and don't see a way to dismiss it -- people were claiming instantly recognizable, impossible-to-miss massive differences in quality for the older violins. Yet nobody has been able to back up those claims with results; all results so far indicate that when the age and maker are obscured, identification by sound alone is no better than chance.


Personally, I do tend to think the difference is much harder to distinguish than tradition would dictate. These studies are evidence in that direction, in fact. But simply sampling 10 people is about as good as 10 anecdotes.

> Also known as ...

Please. What I've just written makes the rest of that sentence silly. There is no need to jump to conclusions.


I agree with your point, and to make it worse some of those 10 people were from a previous, discredited study by the same authors.

Funny enough two comments I made - on this thread, now dead - went from +2 to -4 very quickly. Another comment went from +5 to +2.

I've got a pretty strong feeling that people love telling this anecdote about "science over art!" and ironically don't like it when it turns out the science is actually bad science. As a result, they're banding together and simply mass-downvoting things.

There's an interesting debate to be had here, on the merits of the sounds of different instruments, and the linked NYT article is fascinating. Unfortunately, people seem to be caught up in a frenzy of linking bad science to each other instead of actually discussing violins, which are a personal passion of mine.

I went into more detail on the bad science in my other comment here, which is going negative now: https://news.ycombinator.com/item?id=13233977


Unfortunately what it's going to take is a Randi-style "tell me, die-hard believer, the conditions under which you would accept a conclusion of not being able to tell the difference, and we'll set it up" experiment. And really it will take one per die-hard believer.

But the simple fact is that the die-hards are going to, well, die hard. The result with violins is not at all suspect; it's very much in line with results from other fields where particular products are touted as easily distinguishable due to their inherently much-higher quality. See fine wines for another example.


> But simply sampling 10 people is about as good as 10 anecdotes.

That is not generally true. It depends on what kind of statistical assumptions you make, or statistical analysis you conduct. The term statistical power describes our chance of correctly detecting an effect when there is one to observe. If the effect is small, then a larger number of samples are required to achieve a given level of statistical power, whereas if the effect is large, then fewer are required.

I recommend that you consider the question: if 10 samples are not enough, then what specific number of samples is enough? How do you decide? Fortunately, these questions have been studied in the field of statistics.

Let me give you an example. Let's say I told you that you will flip a coin in the air, and when the coin reaches its peak height, I will shout "heads" or "tails". What would you make of it if we ran this experiment 10 times, and I correctly guessed the outcome 10 out of 10 times? Perhaps you would conclude I really can predict coin flips. By comparison, if I called the outcome accurately only 5 out of 10 times, then you'd probably consider my claim false.

But, consider these two possibilities: (1) what are the odds that my guesses are really no better than random chance, and I've just guessed 10 out of 10 correctly by good luck? (2) What if I really am accurate 99% of the time, but I guessed only 5 out of 10 correctly by bad luck? Statistics allows us to evaluate how likely these things are.

If I'm doing the math correctly, then you'd expect someone to guess 10 out of 10 coins correctly just by chance once in every ~1000 experiments. So to see this happen is not witnessing an extraordinarily improbable event; run enough experiments of 10 coin flips and you will see it.

If someone truly has 99% accuracy, then in almost every experiment they will guess 10 out of 10 correctly. They should guess all 10 correctly 90% of the time. A person who is truly 99% accurate will only guess 5 out of 10 flips correctly once every in every 10,000,000,000 experiments. So it is extraordinarily unlikely that you will see someone with 99% accuracy guessing 5 out of 10 coin flips correctly. It can still happen just by chance, but it's really improbable.

Bringing this all back to the main topic, it is possible for a result of 10 data points to count as convincing evidence against the theory that there is a strong effect, such as that musicians are 99% accurate in discerning the type of violin, while it may be inadequate to evaluate whether there is a weak but still-present effect, such as 51% accuracy. Whether the number of samples is good enough depends on how small of an effect you want to measure, and how confident you want to be in your assessment.

A free book is available online called "Statistical Inference for Everyone" which introduces these topics. https://github.com/bblais/Statistical-Inference-for-Everyone


I don't doubt the study that much but I think the point everyone misses in their contrarian zeal to say 'Strads are nothing special' is that there has been enormous progress in instrument construction in the last 30 years and the actual takeaway from "3 cherry picked $30-40K new violins equal/beat Strads in double blind study funded by new violin maker" is not that 'Strad's are garbage' but perhaps that 'new top of the line violins are effing great'.


Unless there is a better study then this is still a big step up over personal anecdotes.

It's also quite common for humans to think the can tell the difference but can't under test lab conditions. We see the same results in everything from wine tasting to telepathy.


>Eh, I'm going to question her ability to identify the difference between a Strad and non-Strad based on this double blind study

lol! That is a brave statement. As @dcsommer said, sample size is tiny.

When you audition for an Opera (well, her auditions anyway...) were always done blind - ie behind a curtain or similar.

When you practice and play at that level, you notice the little things. Same way I can look at C++ source code, or Java, C#, Objective-C or Node.js. I have to stop and think to explain how I know it is C#. I just know.

Again, I didn't say one is better than an other. Just different...


FWIW I think the issue with higher FPS is that it requires different camera work and acting styles for it to be convincing.


Why would acting styles have to adapt to the frames per second? And how?


I'm not sure about acting style, but FPS has an influence on shutter speed, which is usually set to half the shutter speed (eg. 1/48s for 24fps). This changes the amout of motion blur in the image.


That's an interesting observation. I'm an amateur violin player, but as I've started looking at middle tier instruments, I've already noticed that more expensive doesn't necessarily mean "better." Here's the thing, that doesn't mean the money goes nowhere, it just means it isn't measurably better, but it may mean more specialized and tailored to a particular set of acoustic priorities.

There is, of course, some correlation between more expensive and better, I'm more talking about what happens once you've gotten away from the cheap mass produced instruments and even the decent but unremarkable student violins. Once you get up into a higher level, instruments may get expensive because you need a highly skilled craftsperson to bring out particular qualities in an instrument that may match up to what a musician needs. That sort of thing can't really be mass produced, and because it requires a lot of attention and construction time from a skilled person, it's expensive too.

This holds true for a lot of things. For instance, what's actually "better", dinner at the french laundry, or a damn good burrito. The burrito actually isn't cheaper because it tastes worse, it's cheaper because it isn't as expensive to produce. This actually holds for a lot of things out there.

More expensive isn't always "better", but interestingly, just because it isn't quantifiably better doesn't mean you aren't getting something for your money when you spend more, either. Sometimes more expensive means high quality and specifically tailored for a particular set of criteria.


I've wondered this myself. I majored in music and I know that musicians love their legends. They love to agree that this or that historical figure was a genius or a jerk (but usually nowhere in between) and the same sentiment carries over to instruments. It's basically a perfect setup for bias to come into play.


Is it limited to audio output? What about the playing quality or the emotional aspect of playing a great instrument played by other great musicians?

There are an awful lot of variables, especially regarding listening conditions. And then the strings are going to be different too.

You're definitely the odd man out if you don't prefer a Strad to a modern instrument, that strikes me as an odd thing. If it's so overwhelming then there should certainly be something measurable in the audio or there would be differences due to taste and preference, right?


> the emotional aspect of playing a great instrument played by other great musicians?

Equip: Famous Violin [+5% playing ability from placebo effect]


Heh, I'm pretty sure I had this exact effect when I tried playing a $250k Pietro Guarneri violin. Only it was more like [+200% tone quality], and since my own violin is just a decent student instrument, a good chunk of that bonus was real improvement. But the emotional/"holy shit" aspect of it shouldn't be understated either :)


Yes. You can watch some researchers working an "acoustic fingerprint" in this video.

Linking to the start of that segment: https://youtu.be/eNzRHuqvMxQ?t=50m14s

Pretty cool stuff.



Violins? Okay, here's some of my story:

For a while, wanting to learn well the math for theoretical physics, I was a math grad student at Indiana University. They put me in courses beneath me and courses irrelevant to the math I wanted. For "beneath", they put me in a course in point-set topology. Maybe okay: First lecture I learned that the text was Kelley, General Topology. Heck, as an undergraduate I'd already gotten a copy and a reading course, read the thing, and gave lectures once a week to a prof. One week I explained a chapter and the next week worked exercises from that chapter. Another course was abstract algebra from Herstein's book. Gads: Group, rings, fields, vector spaces, and I'd had that as a ugrad. I asked the prof if he was going to cover group representations, and he said that was "deep". I told him it was also my ugrad honors paper. Really bad was early in a course in real analysis where the prof wanted to see me after class about my work on his first test. He treated me with contempt. The test had been on set theory, and I'd used lower case omega for the first uncountable ordinal, believed that the notation was standard (had had a summer NSF course in axiomatic set theory), was in a hurry and didn't explain the notation. So I explained. Then the prof caught on: My work was correct and, indeed, one step shorter than his solution. I asked what he wanted to see me about, and he said "nothing". Gads. I didn't want to see him again. They said Galois theory to me one too many times. So, I kept doing my teaching, mostly calculus. Gads, that was first year college calculus, and I'd never taken it but taught it to myself and started on sophomore calculus.

Then I started studying Fleming, Functions of Several Variables with the exterior algebra at the back, etc.

I liked music a LOT, still do, but didn't know much, but Indiana University had a terrific music school, and my dorm was next door. In the dorm was a violin student of Josef Gingold and a protege of Issac Stern with a letter of recommendation from from Zeno Francescatti.

Once, for fun, in a practice room, with a pianist and me listening, he sight read the Vieuxtemps Fifth violin concerto.

One afternoon in the dorm, he put his old Italian violin under my left chin and gave me a first lesson. Magic! So, I signed up for a first course, one on one, in violin! The teacher was another violin student: Poor guy, had to start out by showing me how to hold the violin, and after the semester he was soloist for the Brahms concerto in Toronto! He was good!

Eventually I made some progress: Problems: Started too late, too little talent and practice! But, violin is doable and about the most fun can have standing up -- get a voice to permit screaming out to the heavens about the passion of the human spirit!

On violins, one difference is how easy it is for the violinist to get the sound they want. That's a biggie. A listener may never know about any of that, but there's little doubt for the violinist!

Years later that violin student was a well known professional violinist. I called him for a recommendation of a violin shop for routine maintenance on my violin, and he gave me David Segal Violins in NYC who had made his then favorite violin. IIRC he said he liked the Segal violin better than any of the old Italian instruments.

So, maybe, one way and another, there are people now who know how to make some of the best violins ever.

Generally I long since gave up on taking seriously anything in the NYT. But, gee, maybe for anything they publish, someone likes it!

My favorite piece of music is the Bach Chaconne for unaccompanied violin. I made some progress with it. To me the best part is the end of the central D major section; it has a violin scream out passion beyond belief. The piece deserves more than a solo violin and has been arranged for solo piano by Busoni and also for full orchestra. A piano performance I like is

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=sOFflFiLlT8

When my startup is farther along, maybe I'll get some maintenance on my violin and get through the rest of the Chaconne. Just need a lot of practice!


Care to explain how your long r/iamverysmart worthy intro about being a Math genius is relevant to the topic of discussion?


I am not a math genius! E.g., in the eighth grade my math teacher gave me a D and fervently told me not to take anymore math!

The role of the math is part of the story: No way did I go to Indiana University to learn violin! I went there, as I said, to get the math for physics. My interest was in physics and mostly still is. So, part of the story is the bumps and turns in the road that can lead one to something very different than the original goal. Part of the story is how I was well prepared for the math program but, still, didn't like it and, e.g., ended up in violin!

Such things can happen. E.g., there are some Freeman Dyson remarks about why he hates Ph.D. programs and how dangerous they can be to a student's health. Dyson is correct. For one more, there is currently a story at the James Simons Quanta Magazine about a woman who got into looking for dark matter from flashes of light in liquid Zenon gas and early in her career, from the pressures, had to learn to make herself out of titanium, learn to dry the tears, etc.

Well, I wasn't made of titanium: No way was I going to buckle down, nose to the grindstone, ear to the ground, shoulder to the wheel, make the best of the situation, etc. Heck no. I'll go start violin, get a job, and learn the math in evenings and weekends on my own -- for which I did make a lot of progress. And I met my future wife.

So, part of the story is, to heck with the titanium, and don't let the dangers Dyson was concerned about take effect; in a bad grad program, give them the middle finger and do something else.

Violin and the Bach Chaconne are a very long way from mathematical physics, but they did have a role.

It was a story, as in communication, interpretation of human experience, emotion. It was an example that maybe others can understand, compare with, and maybe draw lessons from.

A lesson: Can go for mathematical physics and end up starting violin! Can happen!

The mention that I was well prepared for all three courses they put me in, two of them knew essentially all the material and in the third was way ahead in the first lectures on set theory, don't show that I'm smart but just the outrageous incompetence of some grad programs, and that's part of the story, that is, the outrageous, unexpected bumps in the road.

For a little more, later I went to another grad program and did get a Ph.D., but this time I investigated grad programs before applying. There some data indicated that while I was at the Indiana University grad math department, only one entering grad student in 16 left with a Ph.D. Net, the place was a death march. Gee, IIRC in the Battle of Britain, of the fighter pilots, only one in six died. That, too, along with the Dyson remark and the titanium, could be valuable warnings for others. Or, on drop out rates, many Ph.D. programs make the Army Rangers and the Navy Seals look like "fuzzy, bunny play time". Dangers are part of story telling! Or, when see such dangers, don't go there; change course. If got caught in the danger zone, then back out.


FWIW, it seems like even professionals cannot distinguish between a Stradivari and a new violin... and may even prefer the latter.

"When the researchers totaled up the results, there was no evidence the players could reliably pick old from new. And when players were asked to pick their favorite instrument, the winner was a modern, freshly made violin."

http://www.npr.org/sections/money/2014/05/16/313099219/is-a-...


Reminds me of those audiophile A/B/X tests that always end up in a tie between whatever equipment.


Stradivarius was ahead of his time, but all of his violins have been updated and modified since they were built, such as adding Bass Bars [1].

Since then, his results have been duplicated and/or improved. The name Stradivarius still carries an amazing amount of weight. Researchers have shown musicians can't actually differentiate between newly built violins and Stradivarius:

"The duo asked professional violinists to play new violins, and old ones by Stradivari and Guarneri. They couldn’t tell the difference between the two groups. One of the new violins even emerged as the most commonly preferred instrument." [ 2 ]

Therefore when TFA from NYT makes this vague, unsubstantiated claim "For hundreds of years, the best violin players have almost unanimously said they prefer a Stradivari or a Guarneri instrument," take it with a grain of salt or preferably evidence that mainstream media benefits by perpetuating mysterious illusions for the emotional allure.

[ 1 ] https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Bass_bar

[ 2 ] http://blogs.discovermagazine.com/notrocketscience/2012/01/0...


NYT makes this vague, unsubstantiated claim "For hundreds of years, the best violin players have almost unanimously said they prefer a Stradivari or a Guarneri instrument," take it with a grain of salt or preferably evidence that mainstream media benefits by perpetuating mysterious illusions for the emotional allure.

This story is the epitome of fake news. This is the kind of content it is so hard to filter when anybody on the web can make a serious looking website and publish pure fiction as if it were news.

AI is nowhere near being able to classify these stories. Lots of election stories were similarly challenging. Even humans have to be pretty smart to pick them out sometimes.


>This story is the epitome of fake news

Is "fake news" going to be the new meme here? If you think this epitomizes "fake news", your detector needs recalibration.


I can't even imagine the amount of fear I'd feel if someone asked me to make some modifications to an antique violin worth millions of dollars. It'd be like deploying an update on a Friday afternoon.


The fear would be palpable, but on most of those instruments, the modifications were done in the 19th century, as the modern violin was developed. They changed the size and shape of the bass bar, and the geometry of the neck. Also, the materials and construction techniques all lend themselves to repairability.

Just holding one of those instruments would make me nervous, but in terms of both maintenance and use, they were made to be played. This is just something that people accept.

I obviously don't play a Strad (he didn't make double basses, that's my excuse), but when I got a nice new bass, people asked me if I'd ever take it out of the house, or if I'd use my old bass for performances. I tell people the same thing, it was made to be played.


Hah, I can't compare to that, but I do some minor violin-fixing stuff as a hobby. Recently I spent a decent amount of time repairing a 100 year old German violin, and (at first) it's exactly like you describe: you feel like you're going to destroy everything. Like you're playing football with a Prince Rupert's Drop. But then you get into a bit of a pattern with the instrument, and you start to feel more confident... which funny enough isn't a good thing, cause that's when you screw up.

Make no mistake, I love it though, and the finished product is absolutely gorgeous.


I've repaired a few violins, none of them expensive or valuable. One of them I had gotten at Goodwill for $20 or so with a couple cracks in the top.

Popping the top off of the violin was a little nerve-wracking, but that cracking-hide-glue sound is also satisfying. It went back together nicely (I even used the opportunity to glue a piezo-film transducer to the inside of the soundboard).

Now I don't feel quite so much like the violin I'm holding is going to shatter into a million pieces if I twist too hard on a tuning peg.



At least a deploy you can fix or roll back. Break a Strad or Guarneri, and you're done.


Mmm, not necessarily. Someone took a fireplace poker to Bill Monroe's mandolin. Someone managed to put it back together: http://www.mandolinarchive.com/perl/show_mando.pl?55 (The last picture shows the damage, first one shows its restored state.)


Yes, necessarily, we're talking about the financial value of a Strad. I'm well aware of how fixable wooden instruments are, but break a Strad, and you lose millions. Irrevocably.


Good to see another bluegrass fan on HN! (I'm a mandolin player myself, and I think our fascination with Loar F5s is similar to violinists' obsession with Strads)


It'd be like deploying an update before leaving for your honeymoon!


> Stradivarius was ahead of his time

Is this really true?

I was under the impression that the primary issue with the wood was that it came from a stand of trees that had been through an unusual number of drought years and made the wood unusually dense.

So, while the craftsmanship was excellent, it was a happy accident in the material that made it world-renowned. This is similar to how Damascus Steel was an accident of having vanadium in the raw inputs. And was not to be replicated once that raw material went away.

And, you can say what you will about Strads, but wood has a peak in stringed instruments that seems to be about 30-50 years in. After that, it's slow degradation. Strads are long past their peak.


I think you're misinterpreting the statement "For hundreds of years, the best violin players have almost unanimously said they prefer a Stradivari or a Guarneri instrument". While maybe they don't unanimously state it, that does seem to be the prevailing opinion over "hundreds of years". Just because there are violins that match a Stradivarius doesn't change that.

Please audiophiles don't kill me but it seems loosely similar to the debate over lossless FLAC files and a record. Though I will note that unlike the audio debate you're saying there's no perceptible difference between the two.


Audiophiles have moved beyond that stage. Today they are debating the difference between FLAC files and WAV files, and "audiophile-grade" audio players fully decode the FLAC file in memory before playing it, into a single continuous memory region (obviously, continuous in user-space, in kernel space it can still be "fragmented", but this is too subtle for current day)


I hope you are joking.


I suspect he isn't. Have yourself a giggle at these guys discussing the finer points of FLAC vs Wav sound quality (to be fair, some of them do make sensible points about the ability to tag files and whathaveyou, but still...)

http://forums.naimaudio.com/topic/flac-vs-wav-audio-quality


The only possible way there's a difference is if the CPU used to decode a FLAC file changes the noise emitted by the computer or puts noise on the DAC's power supply that affects its clock stability or raises the noise floor.


According to the link you provided, the bass bars were replaced with bigger bass bars to handle the tension of modern strings. So, it wasn't as if they didn't have bass bars to begin with (which is sort of implied by your wording).

For what it's worth, replacing the bass bar or braces periodically could be considered part of the expected maintenance of some arch-top instruments. When or how often it's needed is debatable, but instrument tops will sag over time due to pressure.


But isn't it true that "the best violin players have almost unanimously said they prefer a Stradivari or a Guarneri"? It seems that's what they say, even if they may not actually be able to tell the difference.


Blind tests suggest that most of the brilliance today comes from the brain knowing it's a Strad, because great modern violins sound just as good or better.

(The other brilliance was being 200 years or more ahead of everyone else at the time!)


These blind tests, again and again, conclusively prove that those listening to the music cannot tell a Yamaha from a Strad. They'll describe the Yamaha in glowing terms until they realize it's a violin made last year, then immediately shit all over it.

It's like wine people who are easily duped by a fancy looking bottle. A true sommelier can identify different wines by taste alone, but for every one of those there's a dozen frauds who pretend they know what they're doing, just parroting stuff they've memorized.

The other troubling thing is the gender of performers is very hard to discern from a blind performance, but when the performer is visible the judges skew very heavily towards men. It's absurd. https://www.theguardian.com/women-in-leadership/2013/oct/14/...


> It's like wine people who are easily duped by a fancy looking bottle. A true sommelier can identify different wines by taste alone, but for every one of those there's a dozen frauds who pretend they know what they're doing, just parroting stuff they've memorized.

I think you're off by at least one, possibly two, orders of magnitude on the relative number of fraudsters.


Listening is different from playing. Or perhaps more precisely, the experience of passively listening is different from the experience of actively listening to the instrument you're creating music on.

As a guitarist, I do a lot of experimentation with picks. I can easily tell the difference between different kinds of picks by sound when I'm playing. But I can listen to old recordings of myself where I couldn't even tell you which guitar I played! Interestingly, a drummer I play with regularly can usually tell when I've switched picks, and has opinions about what picks I should be using. Such is the closeness of musicians working together.

Any of these A/B blind test experiments strongly need to be live. A recording isn't a measure of a violin... it's a measure of the violin, the microphone, the mixing board, the mastering process, the amplifier, and the speaker.


The tests are live. The researchers have really bent over backwards to create fair tests, I think. Someone else in this thread linked to a good summary: http://www.thestrad.com/blind-tested-soloists-unable-to-tell...


In case you read this thread without clicking the link: astrodust's comment might lead you to believe that professional violinists can't tell the difference between factory-made Yamaha instruments and a Strad.

The instruments in this test are not factory-made Yamahas. They're hand-crafted instruments made by top masters. In other words, the best instruments now are finally catching up to the best instruments of a few hundred years ago.

As for casual listeners, I'd believe that some of them can't tell the difference between a Yamaha and a Strad. But professional violinists absolutely can--both when playing and listening.


Blind tests seem to disagree. New but very high quality instruments vs. those considered exceptional and unmatched.


> I can easily tell the difference between different kinds of picks by sound when I'm playing

Are you sure? Since you always k ow which pick you are using it's easy for your brain to fool yourself.


Oh, I'm quite sure.

There are limits to the human ability to discern and judge - see the discussion of blind violin comparison. But they didn't sound the same... they just didn't always have the expected results for "best".

You might have difficulty discerning between, say, Hawaiian and Columbian coffee, but you can easily discern the difference between coffee and lemonade. Except under the influence of certain medications...

The point I'm making, though, is that discernment is situational. It depends on experience with the subject matter, and whether it's direct or indirect. I can easily hear the difference between two guitar picks and make value judgments - IF I'm the one playing. My drummer can tell if I switch picks, because he knows my sound so well, but he couldn't tell you which one is which. Most people could never hear the difference.

When I'm playing a guitar, I can discern tiny things like moving the volume knob 10%. But two years later, listening to a recording, I couldn't even tell you which of my guitars I was playing. It's very situational.


The blind tests that I've heard of are done with a screen that the listeners can't see through, but which does not affect audio quality.

It's not a recording.


"A true sommelier can identify different wines by taste alone, but for every one of those there's a dozen frauds who pretend they know what they're doing, just parroting stuff they've memorized."

Funnily enough, these are my exact thoughts on the state of the software industry and trying to hire good people.


Warning: Joke intended!

> A true sommelier can identify different wines by taste alone

Well, for Chardonnay, if (1) it is dry (low sugar) and crisp (high acid) with delicate flavors, then it's from near Macon, France and maybe Pouilly Fuisse or even Montrachet. If (2) it is sweet (high sugar), flat (low acid), with the many flavors of, say, canned fruit cocktail, then it's from California!

Really, this was my summary opinion from wine investigations some years ago, but I haven't had any Chardonnay in years; maybe California does better now!


Yes! I used "the brain" as a kinder synonym for "placebo effect."


Precisely, which lays waste to this cast-off comment, which sounds right, but is completely wrong: " Stradivari’s and Guarneri’s craftsmanship, copying their wood choice, geometry and construction methods. But their efforts have met with little success."

In fact, their efforts have met with great success and by all accounts within the field, we're presently living in the second "Golden Age" of violin making.


Yep -- players cannot identify Strads by sound, and in fact prefer newer instruments.

http://www.thestrad.com/blind-tested-soloists-unable-to-tell...


The test wasn't to see if the violinists could identify the Strads, it was to pick which violin they preferred to play. The Strads might or might not have a unique tone that's difficult for modern luthiers to replicate, but that's irrelevant if it doesn't match the violinist's preference.

It's also worth noting that it's hard to do a fair comparison because the owners of the Strads might not permit the kind of setup/maintenance that wouldn't be an issue with a modern instrument. Even getting permission to change the strings might be hard.

I think it's pretty impressive that a Strad placed in 3rd place against what I presume were some very well made modern instruments. One would expect something that old to either be significantly surpassed by modern construction techniques or to succumb to structural problems.


> The test wasn't to see if the violinists could identify the Strads

Right from the article: "In the second test all the violins were tested again in the concert hall. At the end of the test the players were given a series of violins and 30 seconds to guess whether it was old or new."

> It's also worth noting that it's hard to do a fair comparison because the owners of the Strads might not permit the kind of setup/maintenance that wouldn't be an issue with a modern instrument. Even getting permission to change the strings might be hard.

That's a wild assumption. If I was also speculating, I'd guess that people buying Strads do proper maintenance in order to protect their investment. Since I happen to know that buyers hire both professional players and professional luthiers to keep the instruments in shape, I don't have to speculate. Strads deteriorate when they aren't played often enough, and most people who spend millions on them know that.

> I think it's pretty impressive that a Strad placed in 3rd place. ... One would expect something that old to either be significantly surpassed by modern construction techniques or to succumb to structural problems.

One of them did, another Strad placed last. It's not just unimpressive, but damning to the narrative, considering the myth and folklore that Strads and Guraneris are unsurpassed and that modern techniques have never achieved the same greatness and perfection that the old masters did.

Your guess may be right, but it is opposite of what the violin world mythology claims, and the entire reason it's a "surprise" that the best players can't identify Strads. Even the OP's article starts on the unquestioned premise that Strads are superior to today's instruments.


> That's a wild assumption.

FWIW A participant (of the 2010 study, not the follow-up you linked) did explain in a blog post that "[the Strads] were loaned with the stipulation that they remain in the condition in which we received them -- precluding any tonal adjustments or even changing the strings"

http://www.violinist.com/blog/laurie/20121/13039/


Yeah, I would expect that (and thanks for the link!) - I'm loaning you something super expensive, please don't mess it up. But the comment above suggested the research could be biased and compromised by the owners not doing maintenance. Much lower probability of that.


Perhaps I wasn't clear in my wording. The modern violins are probably all in top-notch condition -- if a violin wasn't in great condition, it probably wouldn't have been included in the study.

The Strads, though, are in whatever condition they happen to be in at the time, which could be good or not so good, depending on whether the years and previous owners have been kind to the instrument. And if there's anything that ought to be fixed for maximum playability, it might not be possible to get permission to have it fixed. Not because the owners don't care about maintenance, but because the owners are more concerned about keeping it in as close to original condition than how good it plays right now.

For instance, let's say there's a divot worn in the fingerboard from years of string friction. With a modern instrument, you'd just take it to a luthier and have him/her plane it down. With a Strad, you might consider how many times you can plane the fingerboard down before you have to replace it entirely, and whether or not the violin would lose value with a new fingerboard. And you might also say to yourself that that divot was worn into the fingerboard by <some famous 18th or 19th century musician> and who am I to erase that little bit of history? People can be sentimental about old instruments, and doing too many repairs may also be more harmful in the long run than only fixing what needs to be fixed and nothing more.


> Right from the article: "In the second test all the violins were tested again in the concert hall. At the end of the test the players were given a series of violins and 30 seconds to guess whether it was old or new."

You're right, I skimmed the article too fast and missed that. I do think it would have been more interesting to test whether the Strads sound recognizably different than whether some violinists are able to recognize a Strad when they hear it. As it is, we're left unsure whether Strads sound more-or-less the same as modern instruments or whether the listeners just don't know what a Strad sounds like. I mean, I play the guitar but if you asked me to tell you whether some guitar I'm listening to is a vintage Gibson or something made somewhere in Asia last year, I would probably fail that test about half the time. That doesn't mean the guitars are equivalent, or give us much insight into the relative merit of one guitar versus another or whether a singular guitar is representative of its class.

> That's a wild assumption. If I was also speculating, I'd guess that people buying Strads do proper maintenance in order to protect their investment.

It was a criticism of the earlier study I had read some time ago. Since somebody else posted a link, I won't go into it any further.

> One of them did, another Strad placed last. It's not just unimpressive, but damning to the narrative...

That really doesn't tell us anything; that violin might not have been Stradivarius' best work, or perhaps extreme old age had taken its toll, or perhaps there was some physical defect in that particular violin that can't be fixed without destroying its historical value, or perhaps it just needs a new bridge or a new set of strings.

I think the idea that any Stradivarius violin will outperform any modern instrument regardless of circumstances or condition of the instrument is a straw man position.


Ugh, I hate this type of comment (nothing personal against you), and it appears every single time - without fail - whenever the word "strad" comes up.

I'm a violinist, played for 15 years - not professional, but I do know one thing about violins. And that's that they are an art form. End of story. There is no "best violin" in the same way that there is no "best artwork".

Saying that "tests prove that Strads do not sound as good as modern violins" is like saying "tests prove that Mona Lisa is not as good as newer artwork". It's patently ridiculous.

Mostly, I hate the trope because it's like clickbait for "smart" people. You'll see someone scrolling through Facebook. They see "15 gifs that prove Kourtney is the best person ever" and laugh. "How could anyone decide the best person ever based on gifs? That's so ridiculous!" But then they see "Study finds that Strads are no better than modern violins" and they share it.

"Tests" that "show" that Strads aren't as "good" are one thing, and one thing only: polls. They say that "of the people we polled, x people preferred y violin to a Strad." Whether or not the poll is blind only determines one thing, and that's how smug you can be when you share the article on Facebook. Note that the majority of these "tests" are simply asking random prominent violinists their opinion between two unlabeled choices. Sure, it's interesting that the most valuable instrument in the world might only be favored by, say, 40% of the population. But at the end of the day, you can't pick a "best violin" no matter how many polls you run.

My favorite violin is this creaky old 100-year-old violin with holes in it. When I've blindly tested people if they prefer it, or my modern (2008) violin, the modern violin always wins - usually by big margins, up to 80% or so. But the 80% that prefer the modern violin give it plain praise like "nice sound" or "solid tone". The 20% that prefer my older violin say things like "damn, that [violin] sounds so nice", or "wow, it really sings". I don't know about you personally prefer, but I'd rather have 20% that love it than 80% that simply prefer it.

(sidenote, on the topic of art: I, personally, am a big fan of Céleste Boursier-Mougenot. I've seen his works a few times and I'm always blown away.)


I think it's more useful to look at those polls of professionals who are unable to distinguish a Stradivarius from modern instruments.

It's not that modern instruments are "better" or "worse," but if they're indistinguishable, the question is irrelevant.


Every professional is able to distinguish a Stradivarius from a modern instrument. Even a non-professional violinist could tell the difference between a Strad and a Strad copy. The best Strad copy [0] is a recently made violin in Canada for about $27k. That may sound expensive, but I personally know several people with instruments more expensive than that.

You may be referring to the question "which is the older instrument". (Which was asked in exactly one reputable study.) To which I say: why does it matter? Again, that's like asking "what is the older piece of art" when showing people the Mona Lisa. Is it interesting that, according to that one study, professional musicians are bad at guessing the age of their instruments? Kinda, I guess.

If you do want to trumpet that poll though, go ahead - be my guest. But you should know that a big reason why Strads are so valuable is (drum roll)... that they don't show their age.

[0] The instrument in question is a 1993 copy of the Booth Strad.


> Every professional is able to distinguish a Stradivarius from a modern instrument.

The available evidence does not support your assertion, in fact it refutes it. I mean, you can assert however much you like, but anyone who suscribes to the scientific method would disagree based on the studies.

(EDIT: That's not say that I don't "get" why people would want a Stradivarius -- its historicity, its rarity, etc. etc. Sound, however, cannot be the distinguishing feature.)


[edit] hm, +2 to -4 in quick succession on a dead post. That's interesting.

TL;DR The published research is absolute worthless garbage, with trash statistics, re-used participants, and downright inflammatory and confusing quotes such as the following: "Some musicians espouse decidedly nonscientific views, such as the existence of spirit guides". Say what you will about Stradivarius violins - I myself prefer Il Cannone to any Strad - but please do not try to prop up an argument with these worthless studies.

Your note of "Anyone who suscribes [sic] to the scientific method would disagree based on the studies" irritates me. "subscribing to the scientific method" is related to accepting published results in the same way that iPhone sales are related to the number of people who died falling down stairs [1]. Even more irritating, if you actually took two minutes to look for real, published studies you'd notice that the literature is surprisingly lacking for such a blogged-about topic. The entirety of published, close-to-legitimate research consists of two studies. Upon a closer look, one may notice that both studies were published in PNAS through the "Contributed" process, allowing them to bypass most parts of peer review - and critically, select their reviewers. After scanning the headline, you may notice that four of the five authors on the original paper are present on the second. Simply Googling the name of the first paper indicates strong concerns that have been voiced about the experimental design of the first paper, which made even the newspapers that published the second paper call the first paper "controversial".

Then, when you look at the second paper, you'll find a number of concerning things. I'm not sure how many papers you write, but even if the number is small I'm sure you'll agree that "The Team" is not something you'll find often in published literature. I assume one of their cherry-picked reviewers realized this, and thus they added the sentence "Although it is unusual to describe the team..." right after starting their "The Team" section, which made me chuckle. Once you start actually reading the beef of the paper, things start to look much more concerning. 40% of the violinists voiced deep concerns with the study.

What really makes me laugh was the supposed selection process. They claim to have selected a good sample of violinists (and make the, er, interesting claim that their sample is equivalent - yes, equivalent - to the entire population.) Then, only in the Supporting Information - accessible only by a link at the end of the paper in small text, by the way - they disclose that some of the violinists that were used in this particular study were exactly the same violinists used in the previous study. This becomes even more concerning when you realize the sample size is 10. A sample size of 10 is not bad by itself, and can often be justified, but in this case they reused study participants who had played the same violins before, and then in the same breath boasted about the quality of their study participants.

I'm not sure how this paper got published a SECOND time. Oh wait, I forgot. PNAS Contributed Papers. Letting Bad Science Bypass Peer Review Since 1914!™

For a fun time (warning: your definition of fun may be different than mine) you should read the absolutely disgusting commentary article [2]. It's basically an essay by the author on "Why Science Trumps Silly Artists!". It includes things such as hilariously saying that irrational minds will see triangles in Kanizsa's triangles when (and I quote) "careful inspection" reveals none actually exist! Wow! Scientists™ realized Kanizsa's triangles don't actually have triangles?! I for one am glad that we have Scientists to tell us that! There's no way our feeble artist brains could figure that out ourselves! The author then continues to let free-form bullshit dribble onto the page. Pop quiz: Are these quotes from Deepak Chopra, or a scientist with a PhD published in PNAS?

- "Expectations retune neural circuits"

- "Our processing can change perception"

If you answered Deepak Chopra, you're wrong! These are actually quotes from a PhD professor published in PNAS. He goes on to say some downright inflammatory and ridiculous things:

- "The findings should put an end to the outrageously high prices charged for musical instruments" What? I really hope this guy doesn't actually believe what he writes.

- "Because artists rely so heavily on their own experience, studies like this have historically fallen on deaf ears—“I know what I know because my senses tell me so” may be the refrain of those who are skeptical of scientific methods." Excuse me? The singular previous study was discredited by scientists first, then musicians second, because of sloppy science.

- "Some musicians espouse decidedly nonscientific views, such as the existence of spirit guides" What? I'd like to hear this man try to justify the inclusion of this sentence. Exactly what was he trying to accomplish?

I'm shocked and appalled at the current published "research", if it can even be called that. Say what you will about Stradivarius violins - I myself prefer others - but please do not defend the "research".

[0] https://xkcd.com/1425/

[1] http://tylervigen.com/view_correlation?id=28669

[2] http://www.pnas.org/content/111/20/7168.full

[3] for more notes on violin stuff in general, see http://pastebin.com/GCuK322j


I can only guess that the downvotes you're getting are based mostly on the "tone" of your reply -- which is pretty hostile, but whatever.

Ok, so here's the deal: If it's really easy to get these things published, then I suggest (if you care enough, you clearly care a great deal) that you try to get a paper puplished on your double-blinded randomized trial (etc.).

Honestly, it sounds like you might have a beef with the scientific process[1].

Btw, what's going on with your (3) link? Was that your edit buffer or something?

[1] No, it's not flawless. There's publication bias, etc., but so far it's the best anyone's come up with.


My apologies if it seemed hostile. I get worked up about bad science, and PNAS's process in particular is very bad-sciency, allowing anyone famous enough to bypass the peer review process. Nothing about you particularly, or your comment :)

And hah, no beef with the scientific process here. I'm definitely very pro-science, and pro-scientific process. But that also makes me very anti-bad-science, and that paper is definitely a jumble of bad science that somehow scraped by the peer review process because of nepotism in PNAS.

Yep, [3]'s my 3am brain dump. Mostly unrelated but dumped in that comment cause it's the only one I can edit.

And now you've got me thinking about how feasible it would be. If I actually go through with this, I'm blaming you for it :)


Heh, happy to take the blame/accolades for the inspiration! :)

I definitely think there's room for improvement (no particular experience with PNAS) in general -- peer review is very flawed, and there are huge problems with publication bias, the area being so specialized that the reviewers (indirectly) know whose article they're reviewing, etc.

Actually, I think one of the most interesting things (science-wise) to come out in recent years is the meta-science of science-about-science, fuzzy as that may be. I'm guessing you already know of him, but Ben Goldacre has been a great source (for us outsiders) for information on this.


Why doesn't the initial claim that strads have some mythically fantastic sound that can't be beaten also raise your hackles?


Oh, it does quite a bit actually! I just don't talk about it here because I've personally never seen it on HN despite being here (in various incarnations) for the better part of a decade.

Since I find the topic so interesting (and because my previous comments seem to be getting votes), I'll ramble for a bit more here on a few other questions I've asked myself, if you don't mind :)

Are Strads a good investment: In my opinion, yes in some cases. If I was ultra-rich (defined as worth over $100MM) I'd buy one. It's a great hedge against a lot of catastrophic events. Asteroid mining makes your stockpile of gold worthless? Violin still has historic and quality value. We can engineer a copy of a strad that sounds near-identical? Ok, well this specific Strad still has historic value. AI is able to create new instruments, or makes human achievements seem lame: Strad still has historic value.

Are Strads my favorite violins: I haven't heard enough of them in person to tell.

Do Strads have something special in their chemistry: Nothing downright magical, and I don't think there's any one specific magic ingredient. It's probably a combination of things, combined with Stradivari's skill.


Another thing you can add to your pros-of-strads list is that it's not just a historical artifact, but it's a historical artifact that's meant to be used (not just put on display). That, in particular, is unusual.


Since everyone's talking about the double-blind study again and as one of the (probably) few people on HN who's seriously played a Strad, I feel obligated to link to my previous comment on the matter:

https://news.ycombinator.com/item?id=12095164

TL;DR—fine antique violins seems to have a wider range of good tone colors available than modern instruments, but these tone colors take significant practice with the instrument (months, not minutes) to discover.


As many have pointed out the entire premise is flawed, with double blind testing showing no objective advantage in the first place (inspirational value withstanding).

Then another study found competitions are biased as well, with judges rating people differently based on physical appearance and the amount of body english they use while playing.


We already know where the brilliance of a Stradivarius rests: in the listener's imagination.

http://www.thestrad.com/blind-tested-soloists-unable-to-tell...


My grandfather made violins as a hobby.

He would wear 20 year old pants, never ever eat out, grew most of his veggies in a garden.

He would think nothing of spending $100's on a tiny piece of wood from god knows where.


Hahaha! My father makes violins as a profession. He also thinks nothing of spending hundreds on small pieces of wood from strange places. He recently flew to Lithuania and gave some strangers several tens of thousands of dollars for a few piles of wood. My dad does eat out and buy new pants every now and then, though. :P


I thought at least part of this was already known.

The story as I've heard it is that there was a rather dry period preceding Stadivari's violin making, which led to particularly dry and hard wood, both good things for a violin.


The primary reason professionals prefer Stradivaris is that it increases their profile and not because they sound better. Time and time again blind studies show that professionals can't tell the difference or even prefer the modern instruments. But when your resume states that you own a Stradivarius, you will be more highly regarded.


Most people can't discern between a violin and a viola, or an alto and tenor saxophone. The percentage of people who have the refined ears to detect differences between a Strad and other great violins must be unbelievably small.


Sure, but the population of people who are involved in the industry where this matters is a much more interesting population.

The population of people that can recognize good sql vs great sql is tiny as well...


The brilliance of the stradivary is aging over centuries on a base of very good craftmanship. Why does the old stuff taste better than the new? Because it has aged well on a good base.

That means today we can build much more excellent violins with machines, but music is traditional, thus prefers the hand made things first. New machine made violin can never sing as beautiful as old mystery thing of ancient music rituals.


Yes, and in the actual a/b tests nobody could single out the stradivari with any significance. The human performer turned out to be the most deciding factor by several magnitudes.




Consider applying for YC's Summer 2025 batch! Applications are open till May 13

Guidelines | FAQ | Lists | API | Security | Legal | Apply to YC | Contact

Search: